نقش نورون های آیینه ایی در تحولات فرهنگی دالما: ارائه یک مدل شناختی برای اولویت اجرای تقارن در جوامع انسانی

نوع مقاله : مقاله پژوهشی

نویسندگان

1 پژوهشگر پسادکتری، گروه باستان شناسی، دانشکدۀ علوم انسانی، دانشگاه تربیت مدرس، تهران، ایران.

2 استاد گروه باستان شناسی، دانشکدۀ علوم انسانی، دانشگاه تربیت مدرس، تهران، ایران

چکیده

فرهنگ‌های مختلف الگوهای متفاوتی برای منقوش کردن سطوح تزئینی خود به‌کار می‌برند، بنابراین اولویت استفاده از الگوهای مختلف در فرهنگ‌ها متفاوت است و این اولویت‌ها به تابعیت از تماس‌های فرهنگی و یا تغییر زیرساخت‌های اجتماعی، دچار تغییر خواهند شد. این کلیتِ نظریۀ مرتبط بامطالعۀ الگوهای تقارن در فرهنگ‌ها است که سال‌ها توسط انسان‌شناسان و باستان‌شناسان در فرهنگ‌ها و طرح‌های مختلف آزموده شده‌ و مقالات گوناگونی در تأیید آن به‌چاپ رسیده است. به‌نظر می‌رسد تقارن برای فرهنگ به‌مثابۀ ژن‌های وراثتی برای انسان باشد؛ همان‌طورکه انسان ازطریق ژن صفات خود را به‌نسل بعد منتقل می‌کند، فرهنگ هم ازطریق تقارن ویژگی‌ها و اندوخته‌هایش را به‌نسل‌های بعدی همان فرهنگ منتقل می‌کند و هم‌چنان‌که صفات ژنی والدین در فرزند به‌خوبی  بروز می‌کند، ویژگی‌ها و تغییر و تحولات فرهنگ‌ها را می‌توان ازطریق مطالعه و مقایسۀ الگوی تقارنی فرهنگ‌های درحال برهم‌کنش مشخص کرد.‌ اما مسأله‌ایی که تاکنون بدان پرداخته نشده، نقش فعالیت‌های شناختی انسان در این برهم‌کنش‌ها است؛ این‌که کدام تطورشناختی منجر به این ویژگی مهم فرهنگی  برای انسان امروزی شد. در این مقاله ضمن بررسی خاستگاه الگوهای تقارن فرهنگ پیچیدۀ دالما و تماس‌های فرهنگی مرتبط، درپی عامل شناختی تأثیرگذار بر ثبات و یا تغییر الگوهای تقارنی هستیم. نتایج این پژوهش نشان می‌دهد ساختار نورون‌های آیینه‌ایی افراد در اولویت استفاده از تقارن نقش داشته، به‌طوری‌که در طی تماس‌های فرهنگی با روشن شدن نورون‌های آیینه‌ایی الگوهای رایج در سنت‌های فرهنگی دیگر مورد تقلید قرارگرفته و دچار اولویت می‌شوند. به‌طوری‌که در منطقۀ موردمطالعه، تماس‌های فرهنگی و به‌احتمال‌زیاد تجارت ابسیدین هم‌زمان با دورۀ فرهنگی دالما موجب تماس‌هایی بین شمال‌غرب ایران و فرهنگ‌های هم‌افق با آن در نواحی جنوبی قفقاز، شرق آناتولی و شمال بین‌النهرین شده و سبب رواج الگوی تقارنی موجود بر سفال حلف در مناطق داخلی ایران امروزی شده است.

کلیدواژه‌ها


عنوان مقاله [English]

The Role of Mirror Neurons in Dalma Cultural Developments: Provide a Cognitive Model for Prioritizing the Implementation of Symmetrical Patterns in Human Societies

نویسندگان [English]

  • Nasim Faizi 1
  • Hamed Vahdatinasab 2
1 Postdoctoral Researcher, Department of Archaeology, Faculty of Humanities, Tarbiat Modares University, Tehran, Iran.
2 Professor, Department of Archaeology, Faculty of Humanities, Tarbiat Modares University, Tehran, Iran.
چکیده [English]

Abstract: Different cultures have various patterns for painting their decorative surfaces. Thus, preference for the use of various patterns is different in cultures, and these preferences are changed based on cultural relations or changing social substructures. This is the summary of symmetrical patterns theory in cultures that were tested by anthropologists and archaeologists for many years. Thus, symmetry has emerged as a powerful platform to understand the similarities and cultural differences between communities. It is believed that symmetry for a culture can be inherited like the human genes that are inherited from parents like the traits such as height and diseases; Cultural characteristics through symmetry can be transmitted to the next generation. But what is important to us is that so far researchers have not paid attention to the role of human cognition and cognitive activation in these interactions and preferences. In the paper, while studying the origin of the symmetrical patterns of complicated Dalma culture and associated cultural relations, we are looking for a cognitive factor that affects the stability or change of symmetrical patterns. The results show that the mirror neurons structure of an individual plays a vital role in preference for the use of a symmetrical pattern such that during cultural contacts, with the firing of mirror neurons, common patterns in a cultural tradition is imitated and prioritized. It means that unconsciously people show a preference for using such unfamiliar patterns. Accordingly, cultural contacts more possibility was as a result of trading, especially obsidian trading. Thus, such trading simultaneously with the Dalma cultural Period led to contact between northwestern Iran and its cultural horizon in the southern Caucasus, eastern Anatolia, and northern Mesopotamia. As a result of such connections, we witness the spread of the symmetrical pattern of the half pottery in the interior regions of modern Iran.
 
Introduction
Symmetry means dividing a shape or object into two equal parts so that the two sides are the same in every respect. However, symmetry is not limited to this definition. The motor behavior and regular repetition of a motif on the surface, regardless of its shape and type, is part of the concept of symmetry, which has entered studies related to culture to a new stage (Faizi & Vahdati Nasab, 1394). Symmetry is divided through mathematics into 1, 2, and 3 dimensions, while it, as biological and cognitive characteristics of human societies, has left a significant footprint. Anthropologically, symmetry is an important perceptual indicator that has evolved to understand a form and is closely related to the visual function of the brain (Hodgson, 2009). However, the patterns of symmetry created on the surface are the result of the cognitive activities of Homo sapiens sapiens (Wynn, 2002) On the other hand, there are neurons called mirror neurons in the human and ape brains that fire when a person is performing or watching action(Rizzolatti, 2005). These patterns makes the study of symmetry valuable in the analysis of culture and cultural interactions (Brainerd, 1942). From a pattern recognition point of view, archaeologists believe that the early use of symmetry varied in different cultures. It means that the patterns were exchanged between cultures through cultural contact unconsciously, and therefore their chronology is likely (Hann 2003a-c; Washburn & crow1998, 2004, crow 1998; Washburn, 1999). But despite its importance, up to now, little attention has been paid to symmetry in the archaeology of Iran and its nature remains unclear. The principal question of the present study is about cognitive and cerebral factors affecting the prioritization of different symmetries in different cultures. Assuming that human cognitive ability is the principal factor concerning the environment and creating the culture and cultural relationships, we seek to recognize these vital factors in these priorities. In this article, to present the cognitive model of the priority of using symmetry in each culture, while studying the symmetric mathematical model of the Dalma culture and its symmetrical relationship with neighboring cultures, the vision and cognitive principles of symmetry have been studied. It is necessary to explain that the reason for choosing Dalma culture is the complexity of chronology, the possible time gap between the Chalcolithic and the Hajji Firuz Neolithic culture, and insufficient knowledge of the Northwestern cultures in this period.
Dalma Tepe is located in the province of West Azerbaijan in the southwest of Lake Urmia. From layers 4 and 5 of this site, a remarkable collection of pottery has been obtained, which is different from the pottery of the previous period. These pottery are dated after the late Neolithic, Hajji Firuz, and the Middle chalcolithic, Pizdeli, in the Northwest. They are distributed in the first half of the fifth millennium and the second half of the fifth millennium in the Central Zagros. They are common pottery of the early chalcolithic and Middle Neolithic period (Abedi et al., 2015). Therefore, Dalma pottery is identified in the Northwest chronology with a time gap after Hajji Firuz and chronologically coincides with Obaid 3 in Mesopotamia (Rahimi Sorkhani & Eslami, 2018). In recent excavations in the region, this time gap has been filled, and Dava Göz Khoy1 has been identified as a transition period to Chalcolithic (Abedi et al., 2018). Contrary to Solecki’s opinion (1973), we believe that Dalma painted is not the result of Hajji Firuz’s painted style. These sherds are similar only in terms of available materials in the environment, and in the Dalma period geometric patterns are more complex and more regular due to the use of different symmetry.  According to the theory of systems, we do not consider any culture closed. On the other hand, according to the interaction theory, Dalma culture forms a subset of symmetrical patterns of Halaf. The strategic position of Solduz valley due to obsidian trade was the cause of such an interaction and effect. Accordingly, we claim that human mirror neurons in the Neolithic and chalcolithic were fired for both designers and observers when drawing symmetrical bands or two-dimensional patterns. Through these neurons, they transferred the symmetry of other cultures to their own subconsciously.
 
Conclusion
Unconscious activation of mirror neurons while watching a symmetrical pattern causes the patterns to shift from one culture to another or prioritize that pattern. It is the function of mirror neurons that causes different cultures to have varied priorities for painting their decorative surfaces. However, the most important features of mirror neurons are the same important features that are transmitted to individuals and cultures in contact with a culture when they view a symmetrical band or overall pattern. These characteristics include the possibility of unknowingly rapidly imitating a process, interpersonal differences in the performance of actions, and the effect of the environment on the performance of individuals. Therefore, we consider mirror neurons to be the main factor of the non-random distribution of symmetry priorities in cultures. As in this case study, the symmetry pattern of the common pottery tradition (Halaf) is imitated by Dalma pottery makers unconsciously during the cultural contact for obsidian exchange, with the unconscious firing of their mirror while looking at the painted pottery of Halaf culture. Accordingly, these patterns are transmitted based on the activity of mirror neurons and have spread over a wide area of the Northwest and Southwest of Iran during cultural contact.

کلیدواژه‌ها [English]

  • Culture
  • Cognition
  • Symmetry
  • Mirror Neuron
  • Dalma
  • pottery
- آقابزرگ، حسین، ۱۳۷۶، مقدمه‌ای بر بلورشناسی: نظریه گروه‌ها، تقارن ملکوت‌ها و بلورها، دستگاه‌های بلوری، شبکه‌های فضایی، اندیس‌های میلر، گروه‌های فضایی و جامدهای بلوری. تهران: جهاد دانشگاهی تربیت‌معلم.
- برتالنفی، لودویگ‌فون، ۱۳۶۶، مبانی، تکامل و کاربردهای نظریۀ عمومی سیستم‌ها. ترجمۀ کیومرث پریانی، تهران: نشر تندر.
- جوانمردزاده، اردشیر، ۱۳۷۵، «گزارش طرح گرمسیری: شبکه‌های ۲۱ و سد چنگوله سامانۀ انتقال آب». سازمان میراث‌فرهنگی، گردشگری و صنایع‌دستی، (منتشرنشده).
- حیدری‌دستنایی، 1395، «دشت سرفیروزآباد در دورۀ مس‌وسنگ: مروری بر استقرارهای دالمایی حاشیۀ شرقی ماهیدشت در زاگرس‌مرکزی». پژوهش‌های باستان‌شناسی ایران، سال 6، شمارۀ 11، صص: 25-38.
- فرشاد، مهدی، ۱۳۶۲، نگرش سیستمی. تهران: مؤسسه انتشاراتی امیرکبیر.
- فیضی، نسیم؛ و وحدتی‌نسب، حامد، ۱۳۹۴، «کاربرد تقارن در مطالعۀ فرهنگ و باستان‌شناسی». جستارهای باستان‌شناسی ایران پیش از اسلام، سال 1، شمارۀ 1، صص: 17-5.
- ساعدموچشی، امیر؛ نیکنامی، کمال‌الدین؛ مشکور، مرجان؛ فاضلی‌نشلی، حسن؛ و فیروزمندی‌شیره‌جینی، بهمن، 1391، «گاهنگاری نسبی و مطلق تپۀ کلنان بیجار: محوطه‌ای متعلق به دورۀ مس‌وسنگ میانه در غرب ایران». پژوهش‌های باستان‌شناسی ایران، سال 1، شمارۀ 1، صص: 31-56.
- هژبری‌نوبری، علیرضا؛ و پورفرج، اکبر، ۱۳۸۵، «تبیین دوران نوسنگی و کالکولیتیک منطقۀ اردبیل براساس داده‌های باستان‌شناسی قوشاتپۀ شهریری». فصلنامۀ دانشکدۀ ادبیات و علوم انسانی (تهران)، دورۀ 57، شمارۀ 2(180)، صص: ۲۶-۱.
 
- Abdi, K., 2004, “Obsidian in Iran from the Epipalaeolithic period to the Bronze Age”. Persiens Antike Pracht, Pp: 148-153.
- Abdi, K.; Nokandeh, G.; Azadi, A.; Biglari, F.; Heydari, S.; Farmani, D.; Rezaii, A. & Mashkour, M., 2002, “Tuwah khoshkeh: A middle chalcolithic mobile pastoralist camp-site in the Islamabad Plain, West Central Zagros Mountains”. Iran, No. 40(1), Pp: 43-74.
- Abedi, A.; Eskandari, N.; Shahidi, H. K.; Sharahi, I. & Shirzadeh, G., 2014, “New Evidence from Dalma and Kura-Araxes Culture at Tapeh Qal ‘e-ye-Sarsakhti”. Iran and the Caucasus, No. 18(2), Pp:101-114.
- Abedi, A.; Omrani, B. & Karimifar, A., 2015, “Fifth and fourth millennium BC in north-western Iran: Dalma and Pisdeli revisited”.Documenta Praehistorica, No. 42, Pp: 321-338.
- Abedi, A.; Varoutsikos, B. & Chataigner, C., 2018, “Provenance of obsidian artifacts from the Chalcolithic site of Dava Göz in NW IRAN using portable XRF”. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, No. 20, Pp: 756-767.
- Aghabozorg, H., 1997, Introduction to Crystallography: Group Theory, Symmetry Molecules and crystals, crystal devices, space grids, Miller’s ideas, Spatial groups and crystalline solids. Tehran: Iranian Students Booking Agency of  Tarbiat Moalem university (In Persian).
- Alizadeh, A., 2003, Excavations at the Prehistoric Mound of Chogha Bonut, Khuzestan, Iran. Seasons 1976/77, 1977/78, and 1996. Oriental Institute of the University of Chicago.
- Bertalanffy, L.V., 1987, Fundamentals, Evolution, and Applications of General Theory Systems. Translated by: kayumars pariani, Tehran, Tondaar Publication (In Persian).
- Hanson, F. A., 2013, Meaning in culture. United Kingdom: London, Routledge.
- Arbib, M. A., 2011, “From mirror neurons to complex imitation in the evolution of language and tool use”. Annual Review of Anthropology, No. 40, Pp: 257-273.
- Barge, O.; Kharanaghi, H. A.; Biglari, F.; Moradi, B.; Mashkour, M.; Tengberg, M. & Chataigner, C., 2018, “Diffusion of Anatolian and Caucasian obsidian in the Zagros Mountains and the highlands of Iran: Elements of explanation in'least cost path'models”. Quaternary International, No. 467, Pp: 297-322.
- Brainerd, G. W., 1942, “Symmetry in primitive conventional design”. Society for American Archaeology for American Archaeology, Vol. 8, No. 2, Pp: 164-166.
- Brass, M. & Heyes, C., 2005, “Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem?”. Trends in cognitive sciences, No. 9(10), Pp: 489-495.
- Bressy, C.; Poupeau, G. & Yener, K. A., 2005, “Cultural interactions during the Ubaid and Halaf periods: Tell Kurdu (Amuq Valley, Turkey) obsidian sourcing”. Journal of Archaeological Science, No. 32(10), Pp: 1560-1565.
- Caplovitz, G. P.; Hsieh, P. J. & Tse, P. U., 2006, “Mechanisms underlying the perceived angular velocity of a rigidly rotating object”. Vision Research, No.  46(18), Pp:2877-2893.
- Coolidge, F. L. & Wynn, T., 2004, “A cognitive and neuropsychological perspective on the Châtelperronian”. Journal of Anthropological Research, No. 60(1), Pp: 55-73.
- Delougaz, P.; Kantor, H. J. & Alizadeh, A., 1996, Chogha Mish. Oriental Institute of the University of Chicago.
- Donald, M., 1991, Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press.
- Dyson, R. H. & Young, C., 1960, “Pisdeli Tepe”. Antiquity, No. 34, P:19.
-  Farshad, M., 1983, Systems Outlook. Tehran: Amir Kabir Publications (In Persian).
- Feizi, N. & Vahdati Nasab, H., 2015, “Application of Symmetry in Archaeology and Culture”. Journal of Iran Pre-Islamic Archaeological Essays, Vol. 1, Pp: 5-17 (In Persian).
- Ferrari, P. F.; Tramacere, A.; Simpson, E. A. & Iriki, A., 2013, “Mirror neurons through the lens of epigenetics”. Trends in cognitive sciences, No. 17(9), Pp: 450-457.
- Gallese, V.; Gernsbacher, M. A.; Heyes, C.; Hickok, G. & Iacoboni, M., 2011, “Mirror neuron forum”. Perspectives on psychological science, No. 6(4), Pp: 369-407.
- Guberman, S., 2016, “Gestalt Psychology, Mirror Neurons, and Body-Mind Problem”. Gestalt Theory, No. 38(2/3).
- Hamlin, C., 1975, “Dalma Tepe”. Iran, No. 13(1), Pp: 111-127.
- Hann, M. A., 1991, “Classification and Analysis of Regular Geometric Patterns with Particular Reference to Textiles”. Ph.D Thesis, the Department of Textile Industries, the University of Leeds.
- Hann, M. A. & Thomas, B.G., 2007, “Beyond the Bilateral Symmetry in Two Dimensional Designs”. International Associstion of Socities of Design Research the Hongkong Polytechnic University, Pp: 1-15.
- Hann, M. A., 2003a, “Conceptual developments in the analysis of patterns part one: the identification of fundamental geometrical elements”. The Nordic Textile Journal, Pp: 32-43.
- Hann, M. A., 2003b, “Conceptual developments in the analysis of patterns part two: the application of the principles of symmetry”. The Nordic Textile Journal, Pp: 44-49.
- Hann, M. A., 2003c, “The Fundamentals of Pattern Structure: Part III: The Use of Symmetry Classification as an Analytical Tool”. Journal of the Textile Institute, No. 94(1-2), Pp: 81-88.
- Henrickson, E. F.& Vitali, V., 1987, “The Dalma tradition: Prehistoric inter-regional cultural integration in Highland Western Iran”. Paléorient, Pp: 37-45.
- Heidari, M., 2016, “Sarfirouzabad Plain in the Middle Chalcolithic Period: A Review of Dalma Settlements on the Eastern Edge of the Mahidasht, Central Zagros”. Journal of Pazhohesh-ha-ye Bastanshenasi Iran, Vol. 11, Pp: 25-38 (In Persian). 
- Hijjara, I., 1997, The Halaf period in northern Mesopotamia. United Kingdom: London, Nabu.
- Hodgson, D., 2019, “The origin, significance, and development of the earliest geometric patterns in the archaeological record”. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, No. 24, Pp: 588-592.
- Hodgson, D., 2016, “Deciphering patterns in the archaeology of South Africa”. Cognitive Models in Palaeolithic Archaeology, No. 133(5).
- Hodgson, D., 2012, “Hominin Tool Production, Neural Integration, and the Social Brain”. Human Origins.
- Hodgson, D., 2009, “Evolution of the visual cortex and the emergence of symmetry in the Acheulean techno-complex”. Comptes Rendus Palevol, Vol. 8, Pp: 93–97.
- Hojabri Nobari, A.; Binandeh, A.; Neistani, J. & Vahdati Nasab, H., 2012, “Excavation at Lavin Tepe in Northwest Iran”. Ancient Near Eastern Studies, No. 49, Pp: 95-117.
- Hole, F., 1976, Studies in the archeological history of the Deh Luran Plain: the excavation of Chagha Sefid (Vol. 9). U of M Museum Anthro Archaeology.
- Hole, F.; Flannery, K. V. & Neely, J. A., 1969, Prehistory and human ecology of the Deh Luran Plain: an early village sequence from Khuzistan, Iran (Vol. 1). U of M Museum Anthro Archaeology.
- Howell, R., 1979, “Survey of the Malayer plain. Iran”. No. 17, Pp: 156-57.
-  Hazhbari Noubari, A. R. & Purfaraj, A., 2005, “Explanation Of The Neolithic And Chalcolithic Periods In Ardebil Region: On The Basis Of Archaeological Data From Gosha Tepe Of Shaharyeri”. Journal of the faculty of Letters and Humanities, Vol. 157, No. 180, Pp: 1-26 (In Persian).
-  Javanmardzadeh, A., 2016, “Tropical project Report: 20 and 21grids, Changulee Dam, Transmission System”. The Iranian Center for Archaeological Research (Unpublished) (In Persian).
- Nadooshan, F. K.; Abedi, A.; Glascock, M. D.; Eskandari, N. & Khazaee, M., 2013, “Provenance of prehistoric obsidian artefacts from Kul Tepe, northwestern Iran using X-ray fluorescence (XRF) analysis”. Journal of Archaeological Science, No. 40(4), Pp: 1956-1965.
- Konen, C. S. & Kastner, S., 2008, “Two hierarchically organized neural systems for object information in human visual cortex”. Nature neuroscience, No. 11(2), Pp: 224-231.
- Konen, C. S. & Kastner, S., 2008. “Two hierarchically organized neural systems for object information in human visual cortex”. Nature neuroscience, No 11(2), Pp: 224-231.
- Kootstra, G. W., 1978, “Visual Attention and Active Vision from Natural to Artificial Systems”. Ph.D Thesis, Department of University in Groningen, University in Groningen.
- LeBlanc, S. A. & Watson, P. J., 1973. “A comparative statistical analysis of painted pottery from seven Halafian sites”. Paléorient, Pp: 117-133.
- Levine, L. D. & Young Jr, T. C., 1987, “A summary of the ceramic assemblages of the Central Western Zagros from the Middle Neolithic to the late third millennium BC”. Prehistoire de la Mesopotamie: La Mesopotamie prehistorique et l’exploration recente du Djebel Hamrin, Editions de la Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Pari, Pp: 15-53.
- Little, A. C., Apicella, C. L. & Marlowe, F. W., 2008, “Preferences for symmetry in human faces in two cultures: data from the UK and the Hadza, an isolated group of hunter-gatherers”. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, Vol. 274, Pp: 3113–3117.
- Lloyd, S.; Safar, F. & Braidwood, R. J., 1945, “Tell Hassuna Excavations by the Iraq Government Directorate General of Antiquities in 1943 and 1944”.  Journal of Near Eastern Studies, No. 4(4), Pp: 255-289.
- Lockwood, E. H. & Macmillan, R. H., 1978, Geometric symmetry. CUP Archive.
- Losin, E. A. R.; Dapretto, M. & Iacoboni, M., 2009, “Culture in the mind's mirror: how anthropology and neuroscience can inform a model of the neural substrate for cultural imitative learning”. Progress in Brain Research, No. 178, Pp: 175-190.
- Mallowan, M. E. L., 1963, “The'Amuq Plain. Antiquity”. No.  37(147), Pp: 185.
- Mishkin, M. & Ungerleider, L. G., 1982, “Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys”. Behavioural brain research, No. 6(1), Pp: 57-77.
- Molnar-Szakacs, I.; Wu, A. D.; Robles, F. J. & Iacoboni, M., 2007, “Do you see what I mean? Corticospinal excitability during observation of culture-specific gestures”. PloS One, No. 2(7), P.e: 626.
- Oberman, L. M.; Pineda, J. A. & Ramachandran, V. S., 2007, “The human mirror neuron system: a link between action observation and social skills”. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, No. 2(1), Pp: 62-66.
- Orban, G. A.; Claeys, K.; Nelissen, K.; Smans, R.; Sunaert, S.; Todd, J. T.; Wardak, C.; Durand, J. B. & Vanduffel, W., 2006, “Mapping the parietal cortex of human and non-human primates”. Neuropsychologia, Vol. 44, Pp: 2647–2667.
- Orban, G. A.; Sunaert, S.; Todd, J. T.; Van Hecke, P. & Marchal, G., 1999, “Human cortical regions involved in extracting depth from motion”. Neuron, No. 24(4), Pp: 929-940.
- Piechowski-Jozwiak, B.; Boller, F. & Bogousslavsky, J., 2017, “Universal connection through art: Role of mirror neurons in art production and reception”. Behavioral Sciences, No. 7(2), Pp: 29.
- Pullar, J., 1990, Tepe Abdul Hosein: a neolithic site in Western Iran; excavations 1978. BAR.
- Ramachandran, V. S., 2000, “Mirror neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind “the great leap forward” in human evolution”, 29.
- Renfrew, C., 1977, “The later obsidian of Choga Sefid”. Studies of the archaeological history of Dehluran Plain, Pp: 289-310.
- Rice, P. M., 1987, Pottery Analysis. University of Chicago Press.
- Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M .A., 1998, “Language within our grasp”.Trends in neurosciences, No. 21(5), Pp: 188-194.
- Rizzolatti, G., 2005, “The mirror neuron system and its function in humans”. Anatomy and embryology, No. 210(5-6), Pp: 419-421.
- Saed Moucheshi, A.; Niknami, K.; Mashkur, M.; Fazeli Nashli H. & Firuozmandi Shirejin, B., 2011, “Relative and absolute dating of the prehistoric Tape of Kalanan. Site of Kalanan is a middle chalcolithic period in the westrniran”. Journal of Pazhohesh-ha-ye Bastanshenasi Iran, Vol.  1, No. 1, Pp: 31 – 56 (In Persian).
- Sasaki, Y.; Vanduffel, W.; Knutsen, T.; Tyler, C. & Tootell, R., 2005, “Symmetry activates extrastriate visual cortex in human and nonhuman primates”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, No. 102(8), Pp: 3159-3163.
- Sasaki, Y.; Vanduffel, W.; Knutsen, T.; Tyler, C. & Tootell, R., 2005, “Symmetry activates extrastriate visual cortex in human and nonhuman primates”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, No. 102(8), Pp: 3159-3163.
- Sbriscia-Fioretti, B.; Berchio, C.; Freedberg, D.; Gallese, V. & Umiltà, M. A., 2013, “ERP modulation during observation of abstract paintings by Franz Kline”. PloS one, No. 8(10), P.e: 75241.
- Solecki, R. L. & Solecki, R. S., 1973, “Tepe Sevan: A Dalma period site in the Margavar valley, Azerbaijan”. Iran, Bulletin of the Asia Institute of Pahlavi University, No. 3. Pp: 98–117.
- Sorkhani, R. R. & Eslami, M., 2018, “Specialized pottery production in Dalma tradition; a statistical approach in pottery analysis from Soha Chay Tepe, Zanjan, Iran”. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, No. 17, Pp: 220-234.
- Tell Halaf, I., 1943, Die prähistorischen Funde. Berlin, Bearbeitet von Hubert Schmidt .
- Tobler, A.J., 1950. Excavations at Tepe Gawra Volume II, London, Oxford university Press.
- Tonoike, Y., 2012, “Understanding Obsidian Artifact Distribution in Iran Using pXRF Analysis”. Group, No. 9, P: Y398.
- Tootell, R. B.; Hadjikhani, N. K.; Vanduffel, W.; Liu, A. K.; Mendola, J. D.; Sereno, M. I. & Dale, A. M., 1998, “Functional analysis of primary visual cortex (V1) in humans”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, No. 95(3), Pp: 811-817.
- Tramacere, A.; Ferrari, P. F. & Iriki, A., 2015, “Epigenetic regulation of mirror neuron development, and related evolutionary hypotheses”. New frontiers in mirror neurons research, Pp: 222-244.
- Umiltà, M. A.; Berchio, C.; Sestito, M.; Freedberg, D. & Gallese, V., 2012. “Abstract art and cortical motor activation: an EEG study”. Frontiers in human neuroscience, No. 6, Pp: 311.
- Washburn, D. K. &  Crowe, D. W., 1988, Symmetries of Culture: Theory and Practice of Plane Pattern Analysis. University of Washington Press.
- Washburn, D. K., 1999, “Perceptual anthropology: The cultural salience of symmetry”. American Anthropologist, Vol. 101, Pp: 547–562.
- Washburn, D. K.; Crowe, D. W. & Ahlstrom, R. V., 2010, “A symmetry analysis of design structure: 1,000 years of continuity and change in Puebloan ceramic design”. American Antiquity, Pp: 743-772.
- Washburn, D. K. W.; Washburn, D. K.; Crowe, D. W. & Crowe, D. W. eds., 2004, Symmetry comes of age: the role of pattern in culture. University of Washington Press.
- Washburn, D. & Humphrey, D., 2001, “Symmetries in the mind: Production, perception, and preference for seven one-dimensional patterns”.Visual Arts Research, Pp: 57–68.
- Waters, T., 2014, “Of looking glasses, mirror neurons, culture, and meaning”. Perspectives on Science, No. 22(4), Pp: 616-649.
- Wexler, B. E., 2008, Brain and culture: Neurobiology, ideology, and social change. London. MIT Press.
- Wynn, T., 2002, “Archaeology and cognitive evolution”. Behavioral and brain sciences, No. 25(3), Pp: 389.
- Wynn, T., 2009, “Hafted spears and the archaeology of mind”.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 106, Pp: 9544–9545.
- Yoffee, N. & Clark, J. J. eds., 1993, Early stages in the evolution of Mesopotamian civilization: Soviet excavations in northern Iraq. University of Arizona Press.
- Zeynivand, M.; Hariryan, H. & Heydarian, M., 2013, “The Dalma Settlements of Songhor and Koliyaei Plains, Central Zagros’. Iranian Journal of Archaeological Studies, No. 3(1), Pp: 39-47.